Beer Syndicate Blog

Shamelessly Promoting Beer

Category: Beer Inspired Thoughts Page 11 of 16

10 Beer Names You Might Be Saying Wrong

10 Beer Names You Might Be Saying Wrong

The pronunciation of some words is regional like tomato (t’may-toe) and tomato (toe-mah-toe), and basically equally correct.  Other times, the pronunciation of a word is just flat out wrong, like pronouncing the word “supposedly” as “supposably” or “specific” as “pacific”.

But why on Earth would you ever correct somebuddy who pacifically says “supposably”?  Please don’t, because correcting people is one of those Catch-69 situations, like when somebody has ketchup on their face: If you tell them, you embarrass them by pointing it out, but if you don’t say anything, then you embarrass them because you let them walk around with ketchup on their face.  Not to mention, maybe the person is intentionally wearing ketchup on their face.  Like I said, Catch-69.

Likewise, please don’t go around correcting people who say the words on this list incorrectly.

Next Page

Strange Brews: Odd Things to Add to Your Beer That Actually Work

By now, you’ve probably seen beers being served with garnishes like orange slices (Blue Moon), lime wedges and salt (Corona), and maybe even a scoop of vanilla ice cream (Guinness).

But what would you say about a beer served with roasted garlic, dill pickles, capers, cucumber, fresh cilantro, feta cheese, and avocado?

I know, I know.  I forgot jalapeños.

Well at least one place in Tucson, Arizona has figured out how to make it work with downright delicious results.

Usually this is the part where fellow craft beer enthusiasts chime in with something about how a beer should stand on its own, or how adding anything to beer is a malicious act of pure unbridled blasphemy.   And for the most part I agree.  Buuuut, when your base beer happens to be BMC (Bud, Miller, Coors) or any other light lager (in this case it’s Dos Equis lager), you’re kind of working with a blank canvas to begin with.  Not to mention, BMC aren’t exactly considered craft beers.


Beer Cocktail Menu at the Hotel Congress

[The Hotel Congress is not only allegedly haunted, but is also the location of where John Dillinger, one of the most notorious bank robbers in U.S. history, was captured in 1934.  The hotel also serves one of the best breakfast menus in Arizona.]

To be fair, the beer cocktail that the Hotel Congress serves has a bit of spiced tomato juice mixed in with it, so the final product is more of a pimped-out Michelada than anything else (you can even chose your preferred level of spiciness).

But who’s to say adding a salty dill pickle spear with a touch of cilantro wouldn’t stand to improve a can of Natty Light or Milwaukee’s Beast?  I bet adding an orange slice to a beer sounded a touch kooky once too.

And on that note, here are a few other easy-to-make beer blends/beer cocktails that turn out pretty darn tasty (assuming you’re starting with a BMC-type beer).  Just remember to blend the proportions to your own liking:

  1. Beergarita: Pour in a bit of your favorite margarita mix at a ratio of 1 part mix to 3 parts beer. Es perfecto.
  2. Beermosa: Beer + OJ helps keep the scurvy away.
  3. Homemade Diesel: Diesel is the name of a popular mixed drink in Germany which is simply a blend of beer and cola at a ratio of about 1 to 1. After adding cola to a light beer the resulting color kind of looks like, you got it, diesel.
  4. DIY Radler: A Radler is another German creation made by mixing about 50% citrus soda like 7 Up with 50% beer. In a pinch, you could probably get away with adding ANY type of soda pop to a light beer; just be brave and give it a try.
  5. American Shandy: Similar to the German Radler, simply blend in 1 part lemonade to 2-3 parts beer for a refreshing citrusy treat.
  6. 7-Second Fruit Beer: Add any type of fruit juice to a light beer and presto- instant fruit beer.
  7. Tea Beer: Tea-baggin’ has never been more fun! Steep a bag of your favorite tea to any light beer and presto, tea beer.

If you have any weird but tasty beer blends of your own you’d like to share, we’d love to hear ’em, so feel free to comment below.

Cheers!


Entertained? Well, thanks- you’re far too kind.  

Tweet-worthy?  That would be very kind of you

Want to read more beer inspired thoughts?  Come back any time, friend us on Facebook, or follow us on Twitter:

Hi, I’m D.J. Pander.  I like beer.  I also blog.

You Don’t Have to Be a Supertaster to Be a Good Judge of Beer

A few drinks ago, I was Netflix-surfing and stumbled across an old episode of Star Trek: The Next Generation.  Here’s the setup: Guinan, the ship’s quasi-bartender played by Whoopi Goldberg, was combining a glass filled with a blue liquid and another glass filled with a yellow liquid into a fresh glass. [Spoiler alert: the drink turned green.]   Data, the ship’s super-knowledgeable (albeit naïve) android, walks into the bar (stop me if you’ve heard this one)…

A Sketch of Data Having a Beer (Star Trek: TNG)

GUINAN: Hello, Data. Would you like to try something new? It’s a concoction I heard about on Prakal II. I think it’s wonderful but I need a second opinion.

[Data takes a sip.]

DATA: Eighty-seven percent Saurian Brandy. Targ milk and Danisian mead comprise the rest. There is an unusually high concentration of fructose compounds and monosaccharaides.

GUINAN: Too sweet?

Now, I’ve heard of “supertasters” before, but to be able to pick out that a beverage has both targ milk and Danisian mead?  That’s what I call a supertaster!

Tomfoolery aside, we can actually learn a thing or two about how to be a good beer judge from Data, the most super-est of hypothetical supertasters.

For example, did you notice how Data was only listing the constituent parts of the beverage but not making any statements regarding his personal taste?  Well, Data being the non-human, non-emotional android that he is doesn’t need food to survive, but more importantly he can neither like nor dislike anything.  He can only analyze food, but not technically enjoy it.

In other words, even though Data possesses a sense of taste, perhaps a sense of taste far superior to any human, he does not express any personal taste.

So no matter what your level of beer judging expertise is, here’s the takeaway:

  1. When we talk about “taste”, we should distinguish between “personal taste” and the physiological “sense of taste”. Personal taste refers to someone’s private criteria for liking or disliking something, whereas the sense of taste refers to one’s ability to identify features of an object using the tongue in conjunction with smell and the trigeminal nerve which is the nerve responsible for providing sensory impressions of texture (mouthfeel) and temperature.

This is important because beer judges in the U.S. take a more objective approach when evaluating a beer by focusing on judging beer according to a uniform set of predetermined beer style definitions, and not judging according to “personal taste” (like or dislike), which should be kept to a minimum.

  1. One must have experience with a range of foods and other relevant sense-data memories coupled with the ability to recall those experiences for comparative or identifying purposes. Case in point, Data would not have been able to identify Saurian Brandy, targ milk, and Danisian mead had he not already had sufficient knowledge and/or experience of them and had the ability to recall those experiences. When it comes to beer, it’s important that the judge has carefully tasted at least a few excellent examples of the style of beer he/she is judging, and, equally important, understands what criteria needs to be met in order to qualify a given sample of beer as a “good” (or poor) example of the style it’s claimed to be.

This experiential condition suggests that “tasting” is a learned ability, which implies that one can learn to be a “supertaster”.  Well, sort of.

Origins of the Supertaster

Remember, I’m trying to convince you that you don’t have to be a supertaster to be a good beer judge.  In order to do that, we’re going to have to talk a little bit about what a supertaster is (and what it’s not), and also how being a supertaster could be considered a disadvantage when it comes to judging beer.

The term “supertaster” originally appeared in print in the early 1990s in an article by experimental psychologist Linda Bartoshuk in the trade journal Food Technology.  Basically, the term “supertaster” was used to describe a specific subset of people in an experiment who perceived a compound called 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) to be intensely bitter (relative to salt) while others did not.

Those who found PROP to be intensely bitter exhibited two other particular characteristics:

  1. They “perceived stronger tastes from a variety of bitter and sweet substances, and perceive more burn from oral irritants (alcohol and capsaicin),” and
  2. They had comparatively more fungiform papillae (the little bumps that house the taste buds) on the anterior tongue (the front two-thirds of the tongue).

In other words, according to the original definition, if you perceive PROP to be intensely bitter, then you’re a supertaster.   Those who perceived PROP as bitter, but not intensely bitter are called medium tasters (normal tasters), and those who did not perceive PROP to be bitter at all are called non-tasters.

Straightforward enough.

But it’s not just that supertasters perceive flavors more intensely.

Further experiments showed that supertasters also display greater chemosensory acuity, meaning that supertasters are able to detect smaller variations in ingredient levels.

With all of these enhanced tasting abilities, why wouldn’t a supertaster be the perfect beer judge, or any other type of food judge for that matter?

The Case Against Supertasters as Good Beer Judges
(The Supertaster-Effect)

Let’s start with a simple question.  If a supertaster perceives certain tastes at an intensified, perhaps exaggerated, level, how then would this person be able to accurately describe the balance of a beer, or fairly judge the appropriate level of alcohol, sweetness, spiciness (as with capsaicin in chili beers), or bitterness of any beer?  That is to say, if a “normal” taster perceives the bitterness of an American pale ale to be around 35-40 IBUs, the supertaster may perceive that same level of bitterness to be much, much higher and therefore incorrectly judge the American pale ale to be excessively bitter and not “to style”. (Not surprisingly, some supertasters reportedly do not enjoy bitter beers.)

Speaking of bitterness, if you look at what flavors supertasters are said to be able to perceive more intensely than other people, it’s particularly bitterness and sweetness. Although bitterness and sweetness are important components in virtually all styles of beer, there are clearly a variety of other flavors found in beer.  And even though the kind of supertasters we’ve discussed thus far (PROP-sensitive supertasters)  are hypersensitive to bitterness and sweetness, it’s not clear how intensely (or not) such supertasters interpret other tastes like sourness, saltiness, and umami, which may lead to misjudging all of the other flavor components in a given beer, especially when describing the balance of those flavors.

So it’s not just that supertasters might over exaggerate the amount of bitterness in a beer, they might also potentially downplay the level of say sourness in a Lambic or Berliner Weisse, taking points off for the beer lacking the requisite level of sourness and so forth.

Now, if the supertaster phenomenon only affected a very small percentage of the population, we probably wouldn’t be talking about any of this with respect to judging beer.  However, most estimates suggest that 25% of the population are supertasters, which might cause some worry on the scoresheets for those brewers who just happen to have a couple of supertasters judging their beer.

But before we hit the panic button and question the validity of the whole enterprise of beer judging itself including any beer ribbons or medals awarded, let’s clear up one little ambiguity about supertasters.

Who You Callin’ a Supertaster?!?

To be fair, it’s misleading to think of a supertaster as someone who has a superior sense of taste if by superior we mean something like “refined” or “exceptionally accurate” as was implied when suggesting that Data is a supertaster in the example above.   The “super” in supertaster denotes someone who perceives certain tastes more intensely than others, sometimes unpleasantly so, which presents the kind of problems we’ve been discussing with respect to supertasters not being good (accurate) beer judges.

So even though we may think of someone like Data who has a super (exceptionally accurate) sense of taste to be a supertaster, this is not the same as someone who we would consider to be a supertaster as the word is largely intended.

And since the supertaster phenomenon seems to very closely correspond to the presence of at least one particular gene (TAS2R38), if it is purely genetic, in a strict sense you can’t really learn to be a supertaster.  However, as I and others maintain, supertaster or not, you can learn to develop and refine your pallet to become an exceptionally accurate beer judge… Perhaps even a super-judge.

But going back to the original premise of this article [you don’t have to be a supertaster to be a good beer judge], you can now see how it’s actually pretty easy to make a case against supertasters being good beer judges assuming we’re on the same page about what a supertaster is.

And even if we throw in that extra feature regarding a supertaster’s general ability to detect smaller variations in ingredient levels in food, it still doesn’t change the fact that the defining feature of supertasters is that they perceive certain flavors more intensely than others which may also potentially include other tastes besides bitterness and sweetness, which means all the previously mentioned critiques against supertasters being good (accurate) beer judges remain.

Of course if the purpose of a certain beer experiment is to determine if there is a detectable difference between two beer samples, then a supertaster would likely have a distinct advantage.  Not only that, in such experiments, if the detection threshold of the subjects had not been assessed, then the resulting data could very well be skewed, which is why most oral chemosensation [taste] experiments include as part of their design a method for identifying and accounting for non-tasters, medium tasters, and supertasters (or some other similar classification).  Again, this doesn’t affect beer competitions as much because those beers are judged individually with respect to specific style guidelines.

But even though the sole purpose of a beer judging competition isn’t to detect minute differences in two beer samples, we’re still left with the sticky question of whether or not unidentified supertasters who perceive some tastes more intensely are negatively impacting the reliability of beer judging as a whole.

So let’s talk briefly about how beer competitions work starting with homebrew competitions, then commercial beer.

Read more…

 

Page 11 of 16

Powered by WordPress & Theme by Anders Norén